The document below is an htm version of the paper. A more graphically pleasing version can be downloaded using an Adobe Acrobat Viewer by linking to www.personal.psu.edu/nxd10/stratdis.pdf

When Adolescents and Parents Disagree: Changes in Disclosure Strategies and Motivations

Nancy Darling, Katherine Hames, & Patricio Cumsille

The Pennsylvania State University

presented at the Society for Research in Adolescence 2000 Biennial Conference



Introduction:

Differences in the way that adolescents and their parents define legitimate domains of parental control may be one source of conflict during adolescence (Smetana & Asquith, 1994). Direct communication about the disagreement, however, is only one of several actions available to an adolescent. For example, adolescents might choose to confront the issue directly with their parents or they might choose to avoid a confrontation or to disclose only some of the information that their parents might want to know. They might even choose to lie about their beliefs, behaviors, or plans (Darling, Cumsille, & Dowdy, 1998). Such actions can be considered strategic (Goffman, 1969; Elkind, 1980) in that they involve calculations about the parents' potential reaction to full disclosure. As children become adolescents, increasing conflict, improved perspective-taking ability, and increasing autonomy may all contribute to changes in adolescents' decisions about disclosing information to parents and their motivations for doing so.

Research on the strategic disclosure of information by adults (e.g. Daily & Wiemann, 1994) has assumed non-disclosure to be the norm and focused on why adults would choose to communicate particular types of information (including potential sources of conflict). In contrast, the developmental literatures on parental monitoring and lying assume full disclosure by adolescents to be the ideal for parent-adolescent communication and non-disclosure to be problematic (Steinberg, 1990). However one aspect of the development of autonomy in adolescence is an increased desire for privacy and increasing reluctance to disclose personal information to parents.

Questions

This paper addresses the following questions.





Methods

Sample: 120 central Pennsylvania high school students (9th-12th grade) were recruited to participate in a study of adolescent social relationships, leisure, and adjustment. Students were recruited using a snowball sampling technique. The core group of approximately 20 teens were approached directly in locations frequented by adolescents who engaged in problem behavior (i.e. students smoking behind the mall before school or hanging out in or near video parlors after 10:00 on school nights). After receiving student consent and parental permission, adolescents were interviewed and completed a questionnaire. At the end of each interview, students were asked to name up to five of their friends who might be willing to participate in the study. These students, in turn, were contacted to participate.

Procedure: Adolescents were asked to rate 36 different issues according to legitimacy, the importance of the issue, and the extent to which they agreed with their parent about the issue. For items they always or sometimes disagreed with their parent about, adolescents reported their strategy for disclosure or non-disclosure and their motivations for disclosing or non-disclosing. Legitimacy and the importance of the issue were measured during the questionnaire component of the study. The balance of the measures were administered as a card sort procedure during the interview.

Strategic Disclosure:



Issue Characteristics:

Person/Relationship Characteristics

Results

Is adolescent agreement with parents predicted by characteristics of the adolescent (age), the mother-adolescent relationship (responsiveness and demandingness), and the issue (perceived parental legitimacy and importance to the adolescent)?



Table 1: Results of HLM analysis predicting agreement of adolescent with parent on issue from child, relationship, and issue characteristics.
    • Positive coefficients indicate greater agreement.
Variable

Coefficient

Intercept (Mean % Agreement) 0.36
Child Characteristics
Grade -0.02#
Relationship Characteristics
Maternal Responsiveness -0.03#
Maternal Demandingness 0.01
Issue Characteristics
Parental Legitimacy 0.14***
Importance -0.02***

Within Person (Issue) Model

Between Person Model

variance variance
Baseline Model .13828 .01890
Current Model .12579 .08982
# p<.10, * p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

1. 9% of the variance is between persons, 91% is within person plus error.

In cases where adolescents disagree with their parents, when are they most likely to fully disclose their disagreement or disobedience?



Table 2: Results of HLM analysis predicting adolescent disclosure of disagreement or disobedience to parent from child, relationship, and issue characteristics.
    • Positive coefficients indicate disclosure.
Variable

Coefficient

Intercept (Mean % Tell All) .24
Child Characteristics
Grade 0.00
Relationship Characteristics
Maternal Responsiveness 0.03
Maternal Demandingness -0.03
Issue Characteristics
Parental Legitimacy -0.03*
Importance 0.00

Within Person (Issue) Model

Between Person Model

variance variance
Baseline Model .07266 .03254
Current Model .07141 .02962
# p<.10, * p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

1. 30% of the variance is between persons, 70% is within person plus error.



Do motivations for full disclosure vary as a function of adolescent, dyadic, and issue characteristics?



Table 3: Results of HLM analysis predicting adolescents motivations for full disclosure from child, relationship, and issue characteristics.
  • Positive coefficients indicate the adolescent discloses because they feel their parent has a right to know. Negative coefficients indicate the adolescent discloses because they hope their parent will change their mind.
  • Items in which the adolescent did not believe they could get away with non-disclosure were eliminated from this analysis.
Variable

Coefficient

Intercept 2.03
Child Characteristics
Grade 0.17**
Relationship Characteristics
Maternal Responsiveness 0.13
Maternal Demandingness 0.01
Issue Characteristics
Parental Legitimacy -0.07
Importance -0.01

Within Person (Issue) Model

Between Person Model

variance variance
Baseline Model .14005 .61967
Current Model .64005 .22410
# p<.10, * p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

1. 81% of the variance is between persons, 19% is within person plus error.





When adolescents choose not to disclose, when are they most likely to lie to their parents rather than omit information or avoid the subject?



Table 4: Results of HLM analysis predicting adolescents decision to lie, rather than omit information or avoid the issue, when they disagree with their parents and choose not to disclose fully.
    • Positive coefficients indicate greater likelihood of lying.
Variable

Coefficient

Intercept (Mean % Lie) .19
Child Characteristics
Grade -0.03#
Relationship Characteristics
Maternal Responsiveness 0.00
Maternal Demandingness 0.00
Issue Characteristics
Parental Legitimacy -0.07*
Importance -0.07***

Within Person (Issue) Model

Between Person Model

variance variance
Baseline Model .17007 .03213
Current Model .15783 .07517
# p<.10, * p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

1. 16% of the variance is between persons, 84% is within person plus error.



Do motivations for non-disclosure vary as a function of adolescent, dyadic, and issue characteristics?







Table 5a: Results of HLM analysis predicting adolescents motivations not to disclose, contrasting Emotional Reasons with other motivations.
  • Positive coefficients indicates greater likelihood of being motivated because of emotional reasons.
Variable

Coefficient

Intercept (Mean % Emotional) .36
Child Characteristics
Grade .02
Relationship Characteristics
Maternal Responsiveness -.00
Maternal Demandingness .05#
Issue Characteristics
Parental Legitimacy .02*
Importance .00

Within Person (Issue) Model

Between Person Model

variance variance
Baseline Model .05252 .05238
Current Model .05224 .04581
# p<.10, * p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

1. 50% of the variance is between persons, 50% is within person plus error.





Table 5b: Results of HLM analysis predicting adolescents motivations not to disclose, contrasting Fear of Consequences with other motivations.
  • Positive coefficients indicates greater likelihood of being motivated because of fear of consequences
Variable

Coefficient

Intercept (Mean % Not in Jurisdiction) .45
Child Characteristics
Grade -.02
Relationship Characteristics
Maternal Responsiveness -.02
Maternal Demandingness .01
Issue Characteristics
Parental Legitimacy .03**
Importance -.01

Within Person (Issue) Model

Between Person Model

variance variance
Baseline Model .06056 .05265
Current Model .06023 .04703
# p<.10, * p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

1. 50% of the variance is between persons, 50% is within person plus error.



Table 5c: Results of HLM analysis predicting adolescents motivations not to disclose, contrasting Not in Parents' Jurisdiction with other motivations.
  • Positive coefficients indicates greater likelihood of being motivated because they do not think it is in their parents jurisdiction
Variable

Coefficient

Intercept (Mean % Not in Jurisdiction) .19
Child Characteristics
Grade -.01
Relationship Characteristics
Maternal Responsiveness .02
Maternal Demandingness -.06*
Issue Characteristics
Parental Legitimacy -.04***
Importance .00

Within Person (Issue) Model

Between Person Model

variance variance
Baseline Model .04853 .04986
Current Model .04738 .03864
# p<.10, * p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

1. 51% of the variance is between persons, 49% is within person plus error.



Conclusion



Adolescents' agreement with parent, their decisions about what to do in cases of disagreement, and their reasons for choosing to disclose or not disclose vary depending upon characteristics of themselves, their relationship with their mothers, and the issue at hand.





A clear understanding about adolescents' decisions to disclose information to parents is critical if we are to understand the dynamics of parental monitoring and the development of autonomy. As children become adolescents, they spend increasing time outside of the direct supervision of adults. Because of this, parents become increasingly dependent upon adolescents' internalization of parental authority and willingness to obey parental rules in conditions when they have no direct fear of consequences. Parents' ability to monitor adolescent behavior, set appropriate rules, and punish misbehavior also becomes increasingly dependent upon adolescents' willingness to share (disclose) information to them.

One interesting aspect of these results is that adolescents' beliefs about the legitimacy of parental authority predict both adolescents' decisions to tell their parents about their disagreement and to lie. One factor determining which choice the adolescent makes is how important the issue is to them. Contrary to our initial thoughts about adolescent disclosure, adolescents are most likely to disclose and least likely to lie when the issue is important to them. In other words, they are more likely to lie when they consider the matter trivial.

Also interesting is that legitimacy beliefs also discriminate between motivations for non-disclosure. When adolescents believe their parents have a legitimate right to set rules, but don't disclose anyway, it is because of emotional reasons - i.e. reasons indicative of shame or connection between parents and adolescents. When they do not believe the parent has a legitimate right to set rules, they do so for reasons that are either pragmatic (they would stop or punish me) or exclusionary (it's my decision, not theirs). These types of motivations indicate neither guilt nor parent-adolescent connection.

These results support the idea that adolescents' beliefs that their parents have the right to set rules both increases the likelihood that they will agree with them, but also increases the likelihood that they will share information with their parents even when they disagree.